Blankenshiptuttle8475

Z Iurium Wiki

Verze z 19. 10. 2024, 17:40, kterou vytvořil Blankenshiptuttle8475 (diskuse | příspěvky) (Založena nová stránka s textem „The Adult Separation Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (ASA-27) is the most widely used self-report assessment of adult separation anxiety (ASA). Despite its w…“)
(rozdíl) ← Starší verze | zobrazit aktuální verzi (rozdíl) | Novější verze → (rozdíl)

The Adult Separation Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (ASA-27) is the most widely used self-report assessment of adult separation anxiety (ASA). Despite its widespread use, relatively little is known about its psychometric properties, specifically whether it is unidimensional, its degree of precision (or information) across latent levels of ASA, the functioning of individual items in general and of DSM-derived versus non-DSM-derived items in particular, and whether the measure is invariant across gender and time. check details We addressed these issues in a sample of 509 adult women and 407 adult men from the local community participating in a longitudinal study of temperament and psychopathology in children. Two items from the ASA-27 were removed so that the measure met the item response theory (IRT) assumption of unidimensionality. Findings from a graded response model for categorical items suggested that the ASA-27 assesses ASA most reliably at moderate to high levels and that the DSM-derived items were more closely related to latent ASA than the non-DSM-derived items. Invariance tests employing single-factor confirmatory factor analysis models suggested that the measure is partially invariant across gender and time at the unique factor level, with fewer than 7% of parameters freed in both cases; this implies that the means and variances of the latent factors and differences in the observed responses are attributable to true differences in ASA. Future work should replicate these findings in a sample that includes individuals with a wider range of ASA severity and may consider removing additional items that provide little or redundant information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).While age differences in episodic memory are well documented, the impact of age on the structure of memories for real-world events has not been investigated in detail. Recent research has shown that the continuous flow of information that constitutes daily life events is compressed in episodic memory, such that the time needed to mentally replay an event is shorter than the actual event duration. To examine whether this process of temporal compression of prior experience in episodic memory is affected by aging, we asked young and older adults to engage in a series of events that simulated daily life activities while their experience was automatically recorded using a wearable camera. Subsequently, participants were asked to mentally replay these events in as much detail as possible and then to verbally report recalled contents and to rate the subjective qualities of their memories. Results revealed that the rates of temporal compression of events during mental replay were similar in young and older adults. In both age groups, rates of temporal compression were predicted by the density of recalled moments of prior experience per unit of time of the actual event duration. Interestingly, however, the number of recalled moments predicted the subjective vividness of memories in young but not in older adults. Taken together, these results suggest that the process of temporal compression of events in episodic memory is unaffected by age but that the subjective experience of memory vividness becomes less tied to recalled moments that represent the unfolding of events. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).Having an adolescent with Type 1 diabetes (T1D) can be stressful for the entire family. This study examined the impact of parents' relationship maintenance on their ability to manage the conflict associated with their child's T1D, the parents' physiological health (inflammation), and the relationships within the family. Sixty couples and their adolescent children with T1D participated. The couples engaged in a stressful conversation about their child's T1D in their home, followed by random assignment to a 2-week intervention designed to increase the relationship maintenance in the marriage. Results from the home visit revealed that when husbands and wives received greater maintenance from each other the past month, they perceived less conflict when talking about their adolescent's T1D, which was associated with less relational load and lower levels of C-reactive protein (CRP). For wives, greater relationship maintenance was also directly associated with less relational load and lower CRP levels. In addition, the relationship maintenance received was directly and positively associated with parent-child relationship quality for fathers, but this association was mediated by interparental conflict for mothers. Finally, the 2-week intervention reduced parents' relational load and the number of stressful conversations and improved the mother-adolescent relationship but did not significantly reduce parents' CRP. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).Despite evidence linking prenatal psychosocial stress and social support to perinatal maternal and infant health, no study has explored couple conflict behavior during pregnancy as a predictor of subsequent birth outcomes. The current study examines whether couples' positive and negative conflict behaviors during pregnancy predict their stress during the birth experience and gestational and birth outcomes. Forty-seven first-time expectant couples participated in a conflict discussion during pregnancy that was observationally coded. Several months later, following the birth of their child, couples reported on their subjective childbirth stress. By summing medical chart data on gestational outcomes and birth complications, we computed a cumulative birth risk score. Negative conflict behavior was related to higher cumulative birth risk scores, and conversely, positive conflict behavior was associated with lower birth risk, even after controlling for maternal pregnancy symptoms. Similarly, more negative conflict behavior predicted higher mother-reported birth stress, while positive conflict behavior predicted lower father-reported birth stress. However, birth stress effects became nonsignificant after controlling for maternal pregnancy symptoms. Although the pregnancy literature has focused primarily on maternal characteristics, these findings highlight the significance of couple interactions in predicting parental birth stress and birth outcomes. This study integrates psychological, behavioral, and medical chart data to enhance our understanding of how interpersonal factors influence gestational outcomes and the birth experience. These results have important clinical implications for potential couple interventions during pregnancy that can shape fetal development, the labor and delivery experience, and influence child and family health. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).Negativity in the family environment during the perinatal period is likely to have detrimental effects on maternal well-being, especially among low-income ethnic minority mothers who are at increased risk for experiencing postpartum depressive symptoms. With a sample of 322 Mexican and Mexican American families, this study used latent class growth analysis to identify meaningful subgroups of women based on their perceived family negativity reported prenatally and at 6, 12, 18, and 24 weeks postpartum. A 4-trajectory model of family negativity fit the data well low-stable (58%), moderate-increasing (26%), high-decreasing (8%), and high-increasing (8%). Higher prenatal depressive symptomatology predicted membership in the moderate-increasing, high-decreasing, and high-increasing trajectories, relative to the low-stable trajectory. Findings suggest substantial heterogeneity in family negativity, identifying three significant growth patterns during the perinatal period with differential implications for maternal depressive symptomatology at 24 weeks and 12 months after delivery. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).Insomnia-related sleep problems are common in borderline personality disorder (BPD) and exacerbate the core of BPD, emotion dysregulation. Insomnia is elicited and maintained through behaviors that disrupt both the homeostatic and circadian sleep systems. However, it is unclear which homeostatic or circadian insomnia behaviors characterize BPD and exacerbate emotion dysregulation, thus warranting clinical attention in this population. This study therefore investigated whether homeostatic (i.e., abnormalities in time in bed and sleep efficiency [SE]) and circadian (i.e., abnormalities in risetime variability and chronotypes) behaviors characterize and exacerbate emotion dysregulation in BPD group relative to healthy control (HC) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) groups. Participants from the community who met criteria for BPD, GAD, or no psychological disorders (HCs) were recruited and completed measures of emotion dysregulation. They also completed measures of daily homeostatic and circadian insomnia behaviors for 14 days. Generalized estimating equations revealed that the GAD group exhibited lower SE than HCs, and there was a marginally significant effect wherein the BPD group exhibited delayed risetimes relative to the GAD group. Moreover, higher time in bed predicted elevated emotion dysregulation in HCs but lower emotion dysregulation in the GAD group. Higher SE predicted higher emotion dysregulation in BPD. These results suggest that the influence of insomnia behaviors on emotion dysregulation is heterogeneous. Idiographic assessments of the influence of insomnia behaviors on emotion dysregulation are advised. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).Psychopathy is characterized by a lack of empathy, callousness, and a range of severe antisocial behaviors. A deficit to accurately process social cues, which has been widely documented in psychopathic populations, is assumed to underlie their pathological development. Impaired attention to socially salient cues, such as the eyes of an interaction partner, is a possible mechanism compromising the development of social cognition. Preliminary evidence from static facial stimuli suggests that psychopathy is indeed linked to reduced eye gaze. However, no study to date has investigated whether these mechanisms apply to naturalistic interactions. This study is the first to examine patterns of visual attention during live social interactions and their association with symptom clusters of psychopathy. Eye contact was assessed in a sample of incarcerated offenders (N = 30) during semistructured face-to-face interactions with a mobile eye-tracking headset and analyzed using a novel automated areas of interest (e.g., eye region) labeling technique. The interactions included an exchange on neutral predetermined topics and included a condition in which the participants were active (talking) and passive (listening). The data reveal that across both listening and talking conditions higher affective psychopathy is a significant predictor of reduced eye contact (listening r = -.39; talking r = -.43). The present findings are in line with previous research suggesting impaired attention to social cues in psychopathy. This study is the first to document these deficits in naturalistic, live social interaction and therefore provides important evidence for their relevance to real-life behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).

Autoři článku: Blankenshiptuttle8475 (Huynh Eskildsen)